Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Effect of physician attire on patient perceptions of empathy in Japan: a quasi-randomized controlled trial in primary care

Abstract

Background

There is limited quantitative research on the effect of physician attire on patient–physician relationships. This study aimed to measure the influence of Japanese family physicians’ attire on the “human” aspects of medical care in terms of patient-perceived relational empathy.

Methods

This was a multicenter, prospective, controlled trial conducted in primary clinics in Japan. We explored the effects of family physician attire (white coat vs. casual attire) on patient-perceived empathy. Family physicians were allocated to alternate weeks of wearing a white coat or casual attire during consultations. Patients’ perceptions of physician empathy were evaluated using the self-rated Japanese Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) Measure. We used a linear mixed model to analyze the CARE Measure scores, adjusting for cluster effects of patients nested within doctor, age, and sex of patients, and doctors’ sex and years of clinical experience. We used the same method with Bonferroni adjustment to analyze patient sex differences in perceived empathy.

Results

A total of 632 patients of seven family physicians were allocated to white coat-wearing consultations (n = 328), and casual attire-wearing consultations (n = 304). There was no difference in CARE Measure scores between white coat and casual primary care consultations overall (p = 0.162). Subgroup analysis of patient sex showed that CARE Measure scores of male patients were significantly higher in the Casual group than in the White coat group (adjusted p-value = 0.044). There was no difference in female patient scores between White coat and Casual groups (adjusted p-value = 1.000).

Conclusions

This study demonstrated that physician attire (white coat or casual attire) in a primary care setting did not affect patient-perceived relational empathy overall. However, male patients of physicians wearing casual attire reported higher physician empathy. Although empathy cannot be reduced to simple variables such as attire, white coats may have a negative effect on patients, depending on the context. Family physicians should choose their attire carefully.

Trial registration

Japanese University Hospital Medical Information Network (UMIN-ICDR). Clinical Trial identifier number UMIN000037687 (Registered August 14, 2019, https://upload.umin.ac.jp/cgi-open-bin/ctr_e/ctr_view.cgi?recptno=R000042749). The study was prospectively registered.

Peer Review reports

Background

Most patients report that physician attire is important and associated with their satisfaction with care [1,2,3]. Patient preference for physician attire is influenced by age, locale, setting, and context of care [1, 3,4,5,6,7,8]. Reports from several countries suggest that patients prefer primary care physicians to wear white coats [4,5,6]. However, in some countries, most patients who visit a family physician (FP) no longer consider white coats a powerful symbol [8]. Previous research in Japan shows that most patients prefer physicians to wear white coats in a primary care setting [1]. However, one study found that some family medicine specialists certified by the Japan Primary Care Association (JPCA), which was established in 2010 following the merger of three primary care academic societies, preferred non-white coat attire, because they felt that casual attire allowed more empathetic interactions with patients [9]. However, there are no studies on whether FP attire influences relational empathy as perceived by patients in primary care settings.

Empathy contributes to effective general practice consultations [10] and has many beneficial effects in terms of health care, such as improved patient satisfaction, better medication adherence, greater patient enablement, and better clinical outcomes [11,12,13,14,15]. The identification of specific nonverbal behaviors that enhance patient-perceived relational empathy may be important for building efficient therapeutic relationships and optimizing patient health outcomes [15,16,17,18].

One study in a traditional medical clinic in Korea showed that patient-perceived empathy was substantially higher when physicians wore white coats and traditional dress than when they wore casual attire and suits [19]. However, a United States study of a large online sample in an analog medical context that manipulated physician nonverbal behaviors showed that patient-perceived empathy was affected by nonverbal communication (e.g., eye contact), not by physician white coat attire [17]. There is also evidence that, compared with male participants, female participants perceive doctors who express brusque nonverbal behavior as having low empathy. Empathy is a complex, multidimensional phenomenon that includes several functional processes, such as emotion recognition, emotional contagion, and emotion priming [20]. Empathy is also context-sensitive in patient–physician relationships [21]. Japan has a very unique culture that relies heavily on nonverbal and implicit communication [1, 22, 23]; thus, physician attire may play a more important role in patient–physician relationships in Japan than in other countries. In Japan, most patients prefer physicians to wear a white coat because it is considered professional or hygienic [24]. Conversely, white coat attire, with its connotations of professionalism, can be a symbol of a doctor’s paternalism, which may negatively influence the “human” aspects of medical care [25, 26]. It remains to be established whether FP dress style is associated with the perception of empathy in patient–physician relationships in Japan.

In this study, we investigated the use of alternating dress styles (casual attire vs. white coats) in FP practice to compare patient-perceived empathy, assessed using the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) Measure. In addition, we tested previous findings [17] of a difference in perceived empathy between male and female patients.

Methods

Setting and study design

This trial was a multicenter, prospective, non-blind, controlled study conducted at primary clinics in Japan. We contacted 10 primary care clinics in the Tokai region of Japan and seven FPs in five clinics (four private clinics and one public clinic) agreed to participate in the study. The five clinics that declined to participate were all private clinics; the main reason given for declining was that staff expected to be very busy because of the high care demand over the winter period. The experience of the seven FPs who participated in the study ranged from 8 to 46 years. One FP was female. Five FPs were JPCA certified. The FPs’ usual attire was white coats (3), casuals (3), or scrubs (1).

Of the five clinics that participated, three had an appointment system. Patients made appointments by visiting the clinic in person, through the Internet, or by phone. Patients were not informed about the study at this stage. We recruited consecutive new patients (aged ≥ 20 years) in the clinics immediately after their consultation. New patients were defined as those who had not visited the clinic for 6 months or more. We excluded patients with conditions that may have been affected by a request for participation (e.g., anxiety disorder, serious infection, terminal illness) and those who could not complete the assessment independently because of their condition (e.g., dementia, blindness, deafness). We also excluded patients who visited the clinics for routine health checks or vaccinations, because the CARE Measure was originally developed in the context of the therapeutic relationship during one-on-one patient–clinician consultations [27].

The study period was from October 2019 to April 2020. Each FP was asked to conduct their consultations wearing a white coat (White coat condition) or casual attire (Casual condition) on alternate weeks. FPs in the White coat group wore a white coat (Fig. 1); the wearing of undershirts, scrubs, and ties was not regulated. FPs in the Casual group wore a collared shirt without a white coat or a tie. We did not regulate the wearing of trousers, skirts, or shoes. The wearing of a facemask was fixed for each FP during the study period (two FPs wore facemasks; five FPs did not wear facemasks), because it has a negative effect on patient-perceived empathy [28]. At the end of each consultation, the FP invited the patient to fill out a questionnaire. If the patient agreed to participate, the reception staff gave them a questionnaire and explained it to them as required. Completed questionnaires were either mailed to the researchers or handed in to the reception staff in a sealed envelope. Survey participants were compensated for their time.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Photographs of model male and female physicians in white coats and casual attire

Outcome

The primary outcome was the difference in scores on the Japanese CARE Measure between the White coat and Casual conditions. The CARE Measure is a widely used patient-reported measure of empathy that has demonstrated validity and reliability [29]. The CARE Measure was first developed in English [27] and has been translated into Japanese and validated in that language. The Japanese CARE Measure can effectively differentiate between doctors in terms of patient-rated empathy [30, 31]. Patients rated the 10 questions on the CARE Measure from 1 (“poor”) to 5 (“excellent”); there was also a “not applicable” option. In the case of missing or “not applicable” responses, the total CARE value was calculated by multiplying the average score for each item by 10. The total possible score range was 10 to 50. The questionnaire also recorded demographic and social information, including age, sex, marital status, education level, employment status, nature of the problem, and presence of chronic diseases.

Sample size

A previous study found an average score on the Japanese CARE Measure for general practitioners of 38.41 (standard deviation 8.60) [30]. We estimated that 676 patients would be needed to detect a 2-point difference in CARE score, which is sufficient to detect a small to moderate standardized effect size using a two-tailed significance test with a power of 90% and an alpha level of 0.05. At least 38 consultations per doctor were required for the Japanese CARE Measure to differentiate between individual FPs on CARE score [31]. To detect significant CARE score differences in wearing a white coat or wearing casual attire, it was calculated that 100 consultations per FP (a total of 700) were needed.

Statistical analysis

To test the effect on CARE Measure scores of wearing a white coat or casual attire during clinical consultations, we used a linear mixed model. This allowed us to adjust for cluster effects in patients nested within doctor, as well as potential confounding effects from patient demographic variables, such as age and sex of patients, and doctors’ sex and years of clinical experience. We also used a linear mixed model with Bonferroni adjustment to analyze the CARE score difference between the sexes, as previous research indicates a gender difference in empathy [17]. To examine the effect of sex on the primary outcome, we conducted a subgroup analysis of multiple comparisons by sex. Only nominal p-values less than 0.025 (= 0.05 divided by 2) were judged to be statistically significant. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software (version 26.0, IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA).

Results

Study subjects

A total of 731 patients were consecutively recruited to the study; 637 patients (87.1%) submitted questionnaires (mailed: 94; handed in: 543). Data for five patients were excluded from the analysis because of the patients’ age, leaving 632 questionnaires (86.5%) (Fig. 2). Four doctors from two clinics terminated the study early because of the COVID-19 pandemic [32]. The number of participating patients per doctor ranged from 49 to 113 (median: 100, mean: 91) for the seven doctors. Mean CARE Measure scores for each doctor ranged from 31.8 to 41.6.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Flow diagram for trial participants

Primary outcome

Patient characteristics are shown in Table 1. The two groups were evenly matched for most variables, although there was a higher percentage of females in the Casual group than in the White coat group (p = 0.012). Of 632 patients, the mean CARE score for the White coat group was 37.67 (95% confidence interval [CI]: 33.64–41.69) and that for the Casual group was 38.60 (95% CI: 34.60–42.60) (p = 0.162).

Table 1 Patient demographic characteristics

Sex subgroup analysis

In the linear mixed model analysis (adjusting for cluster effects of patients nested within doctor, age, sex of patients, and years of clinical experience and sex of doctors, and sex nested within attire), the p-value of the interaction between sex and attire was 0.072. The regression coefficient for sex was 0.027. We conducted a linear mixed model analysis for sex using the Bonferroni adjustment for 619 patients who provided full sociodemographic information, including sex and age. Figure 3 shows the sex difference between the White coat and Casual groups. The mean CARE score for males in the Casual group was significantly higher than that for males in the White coat group (40.34 vs. 38.03, adjusted p-value = 0.044). We found no difference between the mean CARE scores for females in the Casual group (38.17) and females in the White coat group (38.30) (adjusted p-value = 1.000). To explore the effect of age, we analyzed two groups of patients, one above and one below the average age, but there was no statistically significant difference between the two groups (adjusted p-value = 1.000).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Graph showing the effect of attire and sex on CARE scores. Male patients in the Casual group (40.34) had a significantly higher CARE score than male patients in the White coat group (38.03) (adjusted p-value = 0.044). There was no difference in CARE score for female patients in the Casual (38.17) and White coat (38.30) groups (adjusted p-value = 1.000). CARE: Consultation and Relational Empathy Measure

Discussion

This is the first multicenter, quasi-randomized controlled trial to examine the effect of physician attire (a white coat or casual attire) on patient-perceived relational empathy. We found no difference in empathy between the White coat and Casual attire conditions overall. However, the wearing of a white coat during FP consultations had a significant negative effect on male patients’ perceived empathy.

Previous studies in Japan [1, 24] have shown that most patients prefer Japanese FPs to wear white coats. It is likely that patients whose expectations are met in terms of their physician’s attire will experience more empathy in the therapeutic relationship. However, we found no difference in patient-perceived relational empathy between the White coat and Casual attire conditions. There are several possible patient-related reasons for this result. First, previous research indicates that FP clothing is a nonverbal cue that is perceived less frequently by patients compared with tone of voice, eye contact, and facial expressions [33]. Therefore, FPs’ choice of dress did not contribute substantially to empathy as perceived by patients. Second, a previous study [34] found that more tenseness was reported by new patients in a White coat group than in a Casual group, which suggests that the use of non-white coat attire in patient consultations may help to establish smoother patient–physician relationships. Third, modern patients have become more accustomed to physicians not wearing white coats, as increasing numbers of doctors do not wear white coats owing to concerns about contamination [9, 35,36,37]. From the physician’s perspective, a white coat confers professional identity at the expense of personhood, and so is not necessarily empathetic [35]. Our results differed from previous research in Korea which showed that patients’ perception of empathy was substantially higher when a traditional Korean medicine doctor wore a white coat or traditional attire than when they wore casual attire or suits [19]. Patient-perceived empathy may differ according to cultural differences and type of medical professional.

We also found that male patients were significantly more affected than female patients by perceived physician empathy when their physicians wore casual attire. There was a 2-point difference in the CARE Measure score, which is greater than the difference observed in previous studies with and without facemasks [28]. In a previous study that investigated gender differences in an emotion attribution task using functional magnetic resonance imaging, women and men relied on different strategies when assessing their own emotions in response to other people [38]. Previous research using the CARE Measure has also shown that female patients are more attuned than male patients to empathy signals such as lack of eye contact and unequal eye-levels [17]. Women are generally more sensitive than men to empathy and the feelings of others [21, 39]. Women are faster and more accurate at recognizing facial expressions than men [20], better at recognizing emotions, and express themselves more easily [39]. Female patients may be affected by features that are more salient than physician attire, such as tone of voice, eye contact, and facial expression [33]. However, the empathetic responses of male patients tend to be more influenced by contextual cues than those of female patients [20]. Men are also more responsive to threatening cues (dominant, violent, or aggressive cues) [39]. A white coat may be perceived as indicating medical paternalism [40], and so may affect the perceived empathy of male patients more than that of females. Although intriguing, further research is needed to explore such differences between male and female patients, as this was a secondary analysis in the present study.

This is the first multicenter, prospective controlled trial in primary care clinics to explore the differential effect of wearing a white coat or casual attire on empathy. One strength of the present study is that, to reduce information bias (and with the permission of our ethics committee), we explained to patients that the research was about empathy, but did not reveal that we were investigating the effect of physician attire. Our study has several limitations. First, for pragmatic reasons, patients were allocated on a weekly basis and there was no randomization. Second, the study design meant that the study was non-blind. Third, we did not reach the target sample size because we had to terminate the study early owing to the COVID-19 pandemic. This makes it difficult to draw firm conclusions from the findings. Fourth, a previous study identified a weak positive association between CARE score and consultation length, satisfaction with consultation length, and how well the patient knew the doctor [30]. We did not evaluate consultation length and satisfaction with the length, so we could not adjust the results. However, as our study targeted new patients, it was unlikely that the findings were affected by how well the patient knew the doctor. Fifth, the FPs in this study may not necessarily be representative of all Japanese FPs. Certification of FPs is changing in Japan. The JPCA began to certify FPs as “JPCA-certified family physicians” in 2010 [41] and the number of JPCA-certified FPs was only 900 as of September 30, 2020 [42]. From 2018, the certification changed to be a specialty based on the acquisition of general practitioner board certification [43, 44]. Therefore, most physicians currently working as FPs are not well-trained certified FPs and do not necessarily follow the global standard of primary care physicians [45]. For these reasons, statistical data for physicians working as FPs are not available. However, the participants of this study currently work as FPs, and we believe that they are fairly representative of FPs in Japan. Of the seven FP participants, six work in private clinics; this is close to the national situation, as more than 95% of medical clinics in Japan are private clinics [46]. Sixth, we did not regulate clothing worn under the white coat. This may have been a confounding variable, because a patient’s impression of a physician changes according to what the physician wears under his or her white coat [1]. Seventh, the effect of physician sex was not assessed because only one female physician participated in this study (below our target number for females). As perceived changes in facial expression are affected by the gender of both the source and recipient [47], more research is needed on this topic.

The present results suggest that physicians should be advised that wearing a white coat or casual attire does not have an overall effect on the establishment of patient–physician relationships in Japan, but that casual attire may have a positive effect on male patients. Additionally, white coat attire is associated with several problems, such as white coat hypertension [48, 49] and bacterial dissemination [36, 37]. Given these findings, it is perhaps time for physicians to consider alternatives to white coat attire.

Conclusion

We found no difference overall in the effect of white coats and casual attire on patient-perceived relational empathy in primary care consultations, but male patients of physicians who wore casual attire reported higher physician empathy. Empathy is a complex, multidimensional phenomenon that is context-sensitive in patient–physician relationships and cannot be reduced to simple variables such as attire. White coats may have a negative effect on patients, depending on the context. FPs should choose their attire carefully.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to confidentiality but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

CARE:

Consultation and Relational Empathy

FP:

Family physician

JPCA:

Japan Primary Care Association

References

  1. Kamata K, Kuriyama A, Chopra V, Saint S, Houchens N, Petrilli CM, et al. Patient preferences for physician attire: a multicenter study in Japan. J Hosp Med. 2020;15:e1-7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.12788/jhm.3350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Petrilli CM, Mack M, Petrilli JJ, Hickner A, Saint S, Chopra V. Understanding the role of physician attire on patient perceptions: a systematic review of the literature–targeting attire to improve likelihood of rapport (TAILOR) investigators. BMJ Open. 2015;5:e006578. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmjopen-2014-006578.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Zollinger M, Houchens N, Chopra V, Clack L, Schreiber PW, Kuhn L, et al. Understanding patient preference for physician attire in ambulatory clinics: a cross-sectional observational study. BMJ Open. 2019;9:e026009. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-026009.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Petrilli CM, Saint S, Jennings JJ, Caruso A, Kuhn L, Snyder A, et al. Understanding patient preference for physician attire: a cross-sectional observational study of 10 academic medical centres in the USA. BMJ Open. 2018;8:e021239. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmjopen-2017-021239.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Zahrina AZ, Haymond P, Rosanna P, Ho R, Rajini AR, Low BT, et al. Does the attire of a primary care physician affect patients’ perceptions and their levels of trust in the doctor? Malays FamPhys. 2018;13:3–11.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Batais MA. Patients’ attitudes toward the attire of male physicians: a single-center study in Saudi Arabia. Ann Saudi Med. 2014;34:383–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.5144/0256-4947.2014.383.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Palazzo S, Hocken DB. Patients’ perspectives on how doctors dress. J Hosp Infect. 2010;74:30–4. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jhin.2009.08.021.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Sebo P, Herrmann FR, Haller DM. White coat in primary care: what do patients think today? Cross-sectional study. Swiss Med Wkly. 2014;144:w14072. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4414/smw.2014.14072.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Yoshikawa Y, Matsuhisa T, Takahashi N, Sato J, Ban N. A survey of Japanese physician preference for attire: what to wear and why. Nagoya J Med Sci. 2020;82:735–45.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Mercer SW, Reynolds WJ. Empathy and quality of care. Br J Gen Pract. 2002;52(Suppl):S9-12.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Derksen F, Bensing J, Lagro-Janssen A. Effectiveness of empathy in general practice: a systematic review. Br J Gen Pract. 2013;63:e76-84. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3399/bjgp13X660814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Mercer SW, Jani BD, Maxwell M, Wong SY, Watt GC. Patient enablement requires physician empathy: a cross-sectional study of general practice consultations in areas of high and low socioeconomic deprivation in Scotland. BMC Fam Pract. 2012;13:6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2296-13-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Kim SS, Kaplowitz S, Johnston MV. The effects of physician empathy on patient satisfaction and compliance. Eval Health Prof. 2004;27:237–51. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/0163278704267037.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Hojat M, Louis DZ, Markham FW, Wender R, Rabinowitz C, Gonnella JS. Physicians’ empathy and clinical outcomes for diabetic patients. Acad Med. 2011;86:359–64. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1097/ACM.0b013e3182086fe1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Mercer SW, Higgins M, Bikker AM, Fitzpatrick B, McConnachie A, Lloyd SM, et al. General practitioners’ empathy and health outcomes: a prospective observational study of consultations in areas of high and low deprivation. Ann Fam Med. 2016;14:117–24. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1370/afm.1910.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Riess H, Kraft-Todd G. Empathy: a tool to enhance nonverbal communication between clinicians and their patients. Acad Med. 2014;89:1108–12. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1097/ACM.0000000000000287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Kraft-Todd GT, Reinero DA, Kelley JM, Heberlein AS, Baer L, Riess H. Empathic nonverbal behavior increases ratings of both warmth and competence in a medical context. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0177758. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1371/journal.pone.0177758.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Little P, White P, Kelly J, Everitt H, Gashi S, Bikker A, et al. Verbal and non-verbal behaviour and patient perception of communication in primary care: an observational study. Br J Gen Pract. 2015;65:e357–65. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3399/bjgp15X685249.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Chung H, Lee H, Chang DS, Kim HS, Lee H, Park HJ, et al. Doctor’s attire influences perceived empathy in the patient-doctor relationship. Patient Educ Couns. 2012;89:387–91. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.pec.2012.02.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Christov-Moore L, Simpson EA, Coude G, Grigaityte K, Iacoboni M, Ferrari PF. Empathy: gender effects in brain and behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;46(Pt 4):604–27. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Hojat M. Empathy in health professions, education, and patient care. Springer International Publishing; 2016.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  22. Nishimura S, Nevgi A, Tella S. Communication style and cultural features in high/low context communication cultures: a case study of Finland, Japan and India. Teoksessa A Kallioniemi (toim), Uudistuva ja kehittyvä ainedidaktiikka Ainedidaktinen symposium. 2008; 8:783–96. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.13140/RG.2.2.31338.06084

  23. Richardson RM, Smith SW. The influence of high/low-context culture and power distance on choice of communication media: students’ media choice to communicate with professors in Japan and America. Int J Intercult Relat. 2007;31:479–501. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2007.01.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Yamada Y, Takahashi O, Ohde S, Deshpande GA, Fukui T. Patients’ preferences for doctors’ attire in Japan. Intern Med. 2010;49:1521–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.2169/internalmedicine.49.3572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Goldberg JL. Humanism or professionalism? The white coat ceremony and medical education. Acad Med. 2008;83:715–22. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1097/ACM.0b013e31817eba30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Lill MM, Wilkinson TJ. Judging a book by its cover: descriptive survey of patients’ preferences for doctors’ appearance and mode of address. BMJ. 2005;331:1524–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmj.331.7531.1524.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Mercer SW, Maxwell M, Heaney D, Watt GC. The consultation and relational empathy (CARE) measure: development and preliminary validation and reliability of an empathy-based consultation process measure. Fam Pract. 2004;21:699–705. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/fampra/cmh621.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Wong CK, Yip BH, Mercer S, Griffiths S, Kung K, Wong MC, et al. Effect of facemasks on empathy and relational continuity: a randomised controlled trial in primary care. BMC Fam Pract. 2013;14:200. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2296-14-200.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Stepien KA, Baernstein A. Educating for empathy. A review. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21:524–30. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00443.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Aomatsu M, Abe H, Abe K, Yasui H, Suzuki T, Sato J, et al. Validity and reliability of the Japanese version of the CARE measure in a general medicine outpatient setting. Fam Pract. 2014;31:118–26. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/fampra/cmt053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Matsuhisa T, Takahashi N, Aomatsu M, Takahashi K, Nishino J, Ban N, et al. How many patients are required to provide a high level of reliability in the Japanese version of the CARE Measure? A secondary analysis. BMC FamPract. 2018;19:138. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12875-018-0826-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Looi MK. Covid-19: Japan declares state of emergency as Tokyo cases soar. BMJ. 2020;369:m1447. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmj.m1447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Marcinowicz L, Konstantynowicz J, Godlewski C. Patients’ perceptions of GP non-verbal communication: a qualitative study. Br J Gen Pract. 2010;60:83–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3399/bjgp10X483111.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Ikusaka M, Kamegai M, Sunaga T, Narita N, Kobayashi H, Yonenami K, et al. Patients’ attitude toward consultations by a physician without a white coat in Japan. Intern Med. 1999;38:533–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.2169/internalmedicine.38.533.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Wellbery C, Chan M. White coat, patient gown. Med Humanit. 2014;40:90–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/medhum-2013-010463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Fernandes E. Doctors and medical students in India should stop wearing white coats. BMJ. 2015;351:h3855. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmj.h3855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Banu A, Anand M, Nagi N. White coats as a vehicle for bacterial dissemination. J Clin Diagn Res. 2012;6:1381–4. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.7860/JCDR/2012/4286.2364.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Schulte-Ruther M, Markowitsch HJ, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Piefke M. Gender differences in brain networks supporting empathy. Neuroimage. 2008;42:393–403. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Kret ME, De Gelder B. A review on sex differences in processing emotional signals. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:1211–21. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.022.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Russell PC. The white coat ceremony: turning trust into entitlement. Teach Learn Med. 2002;14:56–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1207/S15328015TLM1401_13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Japan Primary Care Association. History of JPCA. https://www.primary-care.or.jp/jpca_eng/history.html. Accessed 17 Feb 2021.

  42. Japan Primary Care Association. Membership. http://www.primary-care.or.jp/jpca_eng/membership.html. Accessed 17 Feb 2021.

  43. Kaneko M, Matsushima M. Current trends in Japanese health care: establishing a system for board-certificated GPs. Br J Gen Pract. 2017;67:29. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3399/bjgp17X688669.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Japanese Medical Specialty Board. https://jmsb.or.jp/sogo/. (in Japanese) Accessed 17 Feb 2021.

  45. Ban N, Fetters MD. Education for health professionals in Japan: time to change. Lancet. 2011;378:1206–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/S0140-6736(11)61189-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare. Survey of medical institutions of 2019. https://www.mhlw.go.jp/toukei/saikin/hw/iryosd/19/. Accessed 17 Feb 2021.

  47. Seidel EM, Habel U, Kirschner M, Gur RC, Derntl B. The impact of facial emotional expressions on behavioral tendencies in women and men. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2010;36:500–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/a0018169.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Mancia G, Bombelli M, Seravalle G, Grassi G. Diagnosis and management of patients with white-coat and masked hypertension. Nat Rev Cardiol. 2011;8:686–93. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1038/nrcardio.2011.115.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Cobos B, Haskard-Zolnierek K, Howard K. White coat hypertension: improving the patient-health care practitioner relationship. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2015;8:133–41. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.2147/PRBM.S61192.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the patients and FPs who contributed to the study.

Funding

This work was supported by a JSPS KAKENHI Grant (Number JP19K19352). The funding body had no role in the design of the study, collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data, or in the writing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

TM, NT, KT, MA, JS, SM, and NB designed the study. TM, YY, and JS worked on the data collection. TM and KT carried out the data analysis. TM wrote the first version of the article, which was then revised by all the authors. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Takaharu Matsuhisa.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The protocol was approved by Nagoya University (approval number 2019–0302). Patients gave their verbal informed consent to participate in the study as described in the Methods section and this was approved by the ethics committee.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Matsuhisa, T., Takahashi, N., Takahashi, K. et al. Effect of physician attire on patient perceptions of empathy in Japan: a quasi-randomized controlled trial in primary care. BMC Fam Pract 22, 59 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12875-021-01416-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12875-021-01416-w

Keywords